You’ve Heard of Dyslexia, But Why Not Dyscalculia?

The inability to process numeric information, dyscalculia is still poorly understood. Finding therapies may require looking beyond the numbers

Illustration of an artist's concept showing a person holding a pencil, performing a mathematical calculation on a sheet of paper while numbers and symbols float around in the space above the paper

Kudryavtsev Pavel/Getty Images

Experts estimate that 3 to 7 percent of the global population has developmental dyscalculia, although it often goes undiagnosed. The disorder may be poorly understood, but it is very real—and so are its consequences.

Dyscalculia, which is classified as a specific learning disorder in the American Psychiatric Association’s Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5), typically appears in childhood and affects a person’s ability to understand math and number-based information. People with dyscalculia process math-related concepts differently than those without the disorder, but that doesn’t mean they are less intelligent.

Although dyscalculia is defined in the DSM-5 and accepted as a disorder, a lack of research has left many unanswered questions about its causes and treatment. People with dyscalculia often struggle with transitive inference—a form of deductive reasoning used to derive a relation between items. (For example, if item A is larger than item B and item B is larger than item C, then item A is larger than item C.)


On supporting science journalism

If you're enjoying this article, consider supporting our award-winning journalism by subscribing. By purchasing a subscription you are helping to ensure the future of impactful stories about the discoveries and ideas shaping our world today.


They may also have trouble keeping track of time, distinguishing left from right and following sequential instructions, so they may struggle to drive a car, follow a recipe or learn dance steps. And they may have difficulty with putting sequences such as everyday familiar events in order.

It’s common for people with dyscalculia to also have mental health disorders, including anxiety and panic disorders and depression, as well as other neurodivergent conditions, such as attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, sensory processing disorder, autism spectrum disorder and dyslexia.

Dyslexia, which causes difficulty with reading and is also classified as a specific learning disorder, is much more widely known and less of a mystery than dyscalculia. According to a 2018 study co-authored by Kinga Morsanyi, a senior lecturer in mathematical cognition at Loughborough University in England, a child with dyslexia is 100 times more likely to be diagnosed and given support than a child with dyscalculia. There are many reasons for this difference, including that dyscalculia is far less understood than dyslexia by educators and researchers (among whom there are no agreed-on criteria to inform diagnosis). To date, research is inconclusive, and there’s a debate around whether number blindness or number perception is at the root of dyscalculia.

Dyscalculia has neither one definitive cause nor one single set of symptoms, and studies exploring its causes have led to differing conclusions. Further, dyscalculia research, scientists say, lags far behind dyslexia research, in part because of a lack of awareness and funding and in part because math (beyond the very basic level) is not prioritized as much as reading.

"There's a staggering complexity to dyscalculia, and if somebody is bad at math, there are thousands of pathways to that point."
—Gavin Price University of Exeter

“In our society, there’s an assumption that some people are math people and others aren’t,” but we don’t assume the same for reading, says Daniel Ansari, a professor of developmental cognitive neuroscience and head of the Numerical Cognition Laboratory at Western University in Ontario. “We need the same attitude when it comes to math,” he says.

Ansari says that many dyscalculia studies, including some of his own, are underpowered and have very small sample sizes. Also, the way dyscalculia is defined in scientific studies varies substantially, and some researchers find the DSM-5 definition too narrow.

Institutions need to be able to label learning disorders to ensure that resources are allocated appropriately to students, Ansari says, but it’s important to think about them as on a continuum. “From a research point of view, looking at the overlap between difficulties is much more interesting than trying to create these separate categories,” he says.

Similarly, Gavin Price, an associate professor of psychology at the University of Exeter in England, who has published numerous studies with Ansari, asserts that views on the causes of dyscalculia vary: “There’s no consensus view. There’s no consensus diagnostic approach or criteria,” he says, and the studies that have been conducted show conflicting results.

Another factor contributing to the mystery is the complexity of math. Morsanyi points out that children typically learn to read within a few months after they first start trying, and at that point the skill is mastered. If one wants to master math, however, the process is longer. Each skill level builds on the previous one—multiplication and division, for example, build on addition and subtraction—and there are many more levels of explicit skill knowledge in math than in reading.

“Part of the reason there’s such a challenge about dyscalculia and understanding the condition is that we haven’t had a good understanding of the skills we need when we’re learning math,” explains Camilla Gilmore, a professor of mathematical cognition at Loughborough University.

The true challenge of understanding dyscalculia, Price contends, has to do with the fact that it is a very heterogeneous disorder. “There’s a staggering complexity to dyscalculia, and if somebody is bad at math, there are thousands of pathways to that point,” Price says.

A big debate persists in the field around whether dyscalculia is rooted in an innate disorder of number perception (the ability to readily estimate the number of objects) or number blindness (an inability to recognize numerical symbols and to connect them to the quantities they represent). One of the largest studies to date, which included 1,303 children, points toward number blindness as the cause.

Regardless of whether number blindness or number perception is at the root of dyscalculia, a large body of research has identified other cognitive skills integral to a person’s ability to do math.

Price says there used to be an emphasis on the importance of domain-specific mathematical skills, such as understanding and comparing numbers, being able to match numbers and quantities, and counting and retrieving number facts (as in simple addition, subtraction, multiplication or division of two numbers). But over the past five to 10 years researchers have started to focus on how these numerical systems interact with domain-general cognitive skills, abilities that are not specific to math, such as executive function and memory.

This is where it gets even more complicated, Gilmore says, because the distinction between domain-­specific and domain-­general skills is not as clear-­cut as once thought. “Although we can distinguish between the two, domain-general skills might be more context-based, so you might be good at them in a particular domain compared to other domains.”

A person may be good at holding nonnumerical information such as words in mind but have difficulty remembering numbers, for example. Or one could have difficulty focusing attention on numerical information but have no trouble focusing in another context, such as reading.

This leads to the critical contribution of executive function skills, which enable one to focus attention, remember instructions, control thought and juggle multiple tasks successfully.

For one, research by Gilmore and others points to the importance of inhibitory control, an aspect of executive function that relates to suppressing distracting information and unwanted responses when doing math. For instance, a person must ignore colors when adding three sets of objects that are different colors.

Understanding dyscalculia is further complicated because doing math involves logic as well as numbers, and multiple studies document a relation between math skills and both nonnumerical and numerical ordering performance. In 2018 Morsanyi co-authored a study that found children’s nonnumerical ordering ability in their first year of school was the strongest longitudinal predictor of math abilities one year later.

In December 2023 Morsanyi and her colleagues published another study in which children between the ages of four and five were trained on number ordering, daily event ordering and order working memory (in this case, the ability to re-create the correct sequence of a list of animals) during a three-week period. Math skills and math anxiety were measured before and after the training.

“We found that if you train children in processing skills, they get better at doing math,” Morsanyi says. “So this would be evidence that ordered processing forms the basis of mathematics—not the only basis of mathematics, because it’s very complicated, but a very important building block.”

To further complicate matters, numerous difficulties are associated with dyscalculia, but not everyone experiences every difficulty, which means there’s an immense number of variables to consider when researching the condition.

The mystery of dyscalculia will eventually be solved, researchers believe. They emphasize, however, that there must be a standard definition and more research with bigger sample sizes. Price describes the current research landscape as patchy and inconsistent. “How do we study it if we don’t have a good definition or diagnosis, and how do we get that definition or diagnosis if we can’t study it accurately?” he asks.

Ansari also advocates for following in the footsteps of genomic researchers and establishing research consortiums that require investigators around the world to follow the same protocol.

“I feel confident that we are already making progress toward understanding dyscalculia and especially building awareness,” Ansari says. “In the end, it’s all about the children who are affected by it.”

Jaimie Seaton has been a journalist for over 20 years and has reported from South Africa, Singapore and Thailand. In addition to Scientific American, she contributes to Smithsonian Magazine, AARP Travel and numerous other publications.

More by Jaimie Seaton
SA Special Editions Vol 33 Issue 3sThis article was originally published with the title “Have You Heard of Dyscalculia?” in SA Special Editions Vol. 33 No. 3s (), p. 54
doi:10.1038/scientificamerican092024-wIxpWmohdzJnrJfzMPFtn